The study explores trends in attendee preferences, factors in destination selection, and how incentive travel...
Research / Conscious and Unconscious Reward Preference & Choice: A Biometric Experiment
by Incentive Research Foundation
In Fall 2016, the Incentive Research Foundation commissioned Flying Horse Communications along with the help of Steve Genco, author of Neuroscience for Dummies, to conduct a first-of-a-kind experiment in the field of rewards and recognition. The experiment used biometric techniques borrowed from the science of neuromarketing. It was designed to test two hypotheses:
Academic research from the past three decades has offered evidence that people prefer cash rewards to non-cash rewards (Shaffer, 2009; Jeffrey S, 2009; Dzuranin A, 2013; Kagel & Roth, 2016). The studies find that when people are asked what sorts of rewards they prefer, most choose cash. Many researchers have hypothesized that this is because cash is the most fungible reward. In other words, reward-earners can decide what to spend it on, and, for that reason, can more easily rationalize and justify the choice (Dzuranin, Randolph, & Stuart, 2013; Benjamin, Heffetz, Kimball, & Rees-Jones, 2012; Jeffrey, 2003, 2009; Hein, 1998).
However, the studies these results rely on fail to factor for the known effects of salience and choice among non-cash rewards (Lou, Hsu, & Sajda, 2015). This study may be the first experiment to examine cash vs. non-cash reward preference under conditions of where the subjects were offered the opportunity to choose personally meaningful rewards.
This study may be the first experiment to examine cash vs. non-cash reward preference under conditions of where the subjects were offered the opportunity to choose personally meaningful rewards.
Behavior science has taught us that what people say and what they feel are often very different things (Jeffrey S, 2009). Various studies have illustrated this. Perhaps the best-known, and to many, most astonishing finding in choice and preference, is the power of default settings. For example, most adults will make a decision at some point in their lives about whether to select “organ donor” when they get or renew their driver license. This choice would seem to constitute an important one for most people. Yet in countless studies, the decision appears to come down to which is the default option on their driver license application.
Psychologists argue that because the choice is so fraught, people accept the default option to avoid having to think about it (Davidai, Gilovich, & Ross, 2012; Ariely, 2008). Supporting this hypothesis is the observation that even when people blindly accept the default option, they are able to rationalize it afterward as though they had given it extensive thought (Davidai, Gilovich, & Ross, 2012).
Examples of conflicting preference and choice abound. Research by Daniel Kahneman supports the notion that people often make choices that they think they should make, as opposed to what they prefer (Kahneman, 2011). Kahneman and others point out that people do not always make choices consistent with their preferences or with that which motivates them the most. He and others have demonstrated that preferences generally map to “System 1” thinking, (i.e., fast, reflexive, automatic thinking) whereas choice normally invokes “System 2” thinking, (i.e., slow, careful, cognitive thinking).
Preferences generally map to “System 1” thinking, (i.e., fast, reflexive, automatic thinking) whereas choice normally invokes “System 2” thinking, (i.e., slow, careful, cognitive thinking).
Kahneman and others have pointed out that System 2 thinking it is not always good at registering emotions; indeed, relying on System 1 might make people happier, more engaged and more motivated (Hsee, 1999; Iyengar, 2010; Kahneman, 2013). They have also found that people are often poor judges of what they will most enjoy or use and that also causes them to make choices that counter their preferences (Kahneman & Snell, 1992).
Most motivation professionals and a growing number of researchers argue that aside from pure compensation, cash as a job reward does not work as well as non-cash rewards in driving performance (Jeffrey, 2017, 2009, 2003; Schall & Mohnen, 2015; Kube, Marechal & Puppe, 2012; Mahmood & Zaman, 2010; Jeffrey & Shaffer, 2007; Alonzo, 1996; Heyman & Ariely, 2004). The reasons are numerous, but rest in part on Nobel Laureate Richard Thaler’s theory of mental accounting (Thaler, 1985). Our propensity to mentally place our money into buckets for various purposes means that recipients of cash rewards and bonuses often classify reward money with funds set aside for mundane purposes, such as paying the mortgage and/or utility bills. This makes those rewards far less memorable and impactful than equivalent, hedonic non-cash rewards.
Most motivation professionals and a growing number of researchers argue that aside from pure compensation, cash as a job reward does not work as well as non-cash rewards in driving performance.
Though science does not yet fully understand the relationships between the unconscious and conscious minds, (Bargh & Morsella, 2008) we know that much of our thinking and decision-making occurs at the unconscious level (Goleman, 1984; Szegedy-Maszak, n.d; Fannin & Williams, n.d.; Kahneman, 2013). It is valuable, therefore, to gain a better understanding of reward preference at the unconscious level.
In our experiment, the unconscious reward preferences of 42 subjects were assessed using biometric and facial coding techniques ranging from eye tracking and galvanic skin response to fixation time and measures of BIS/BAS. These techniques have been used by scientists for decades and though imperfect, they have gained credibility among researchers in the social sciences where they are commonly used by psychologists to gauge intent, preference and sincerity across a wide range of disciplines (Alsaadi, 2015). More recently, researchers in the field of neuromarketing have used the techniques, to test people’s unconscious reactions to advertising and their preferences for consumer goods.
Pupil Dilation: One of the main biometric techniques used in our experiment was measuring pupil dilation, which is an involuntary expansion of the pupils in our eyes. This happens when we move from light to dark but it also correlates so consistently with arousal (interest and anxiety) that researchers use pupil size, or pupillometry, to investigate a wide range of psychological phenomena. Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman has identified pupil dilation as among the most effective and transparent means of gauging peoples’ “arousal” when presented with challenges or opportunities (Kahneman, 2013).
BIS/BAS: Another biometric technique we relied on was the BIA/BAS Scale. Many researchers have confirmed the use of BIS (Behavioral Inhibition System) and BAS (Behavioral Activation System) as among the most widely accepted measures of what causes various behavior (Wikipedia, n.d.). This measurement shows an individuals natural pre-disposition to either be more strongly drawn toward positive reinforcement (awards) or away from negative reinforcements (punishments).
Our experiment was directed by primary investigator, Steven Genco, a pioneer in the field of neuromarketing. Dr. Genco and his associates selected biometric techniques to fit the intent and constraints of the experiment. Subjects represented multiple genders, ages, and job types (sales/non sales). They were also assessed for personality differences (BIS/BAS). After being prompted with a work reward scenario, their presentation preferences (pupil dilation, eye tracking) and intrinsic reward preferences (facial expressions, eye tracking GSR peaks, and pupil dilation) were measured. The researchers also measured the time it took people to choose their reward (time-to-choice) and conducted qualitative interviews with each to better understand their financial and lifecycle situations, their reward preferences, and why they ultimately chose one type of reward over another.
Our subjects were shown a range of sixteen rewards divided into four categories: “Experience” (sporting event, spa, dinner for ten, concert); “Merchandise” (big screen TV, drone, watch, grill); “Gift Card” (general retail, Amazon, Best Buy, gas), and “Travel” (resort, executive retreat, family vacation, cruise). It is important to remember that subjects in the experiment were repeatedly exposed to digital photographs representing the 16 non-cash rewards and one “cash” reward. Salience was increased further by immersing subjects in four recorded award presentation scenarios, the results of which are described in Part Two of this paper.
Our subjects were shown a range of sixteen rewards divided into four categories: experience, merchandise, gift card, and travel.
Subjects were repeatedly reminded that all sixteen rewards and the cash were of equal value. After viewing each option they were asked to choose how appealing each reward was on a 5-point Likert Scale, from “Very Unappealing” to “Very Appealing.” Next, they looked only at their highest rated choice in each category and “cash” was then introduced as a fifth option. The researchers collected biometric data to measure subjects’ emotional engagement throughout.
The two hypotheses were largely confirmed by the experiment. Biometric techniques used to assess unconscious attraction to a range of rewards demonstrate a preference for non-cash rewards at the unconscious, or System 1 level, offering evidence in support of H1. Almost two-thirds of our subjects chose a non-cash reward after engaging System 2 thinking. This is consistent with H2.
We expect future biometric-based experiments involving a larger number of subjects will bolster our findings with respect to H1. Separate experiments should also test reward preference using salience and/or choice versus control groups that are not offered salience and/or choice to more accurately test H2.
THANK YOU TO OUR RESEARCH ADVOCACY PARTNER
During this IRF webinar, we compared data from the 2024 Attendee Preferences for Incentive Travel study to what is really happening in incentive travel programs. We illustrate how attendee preferences line up against planner data as reported in the newly released 2024 Incentive Travel Index as well as actual bookings through Cvent. We explore how…
The 2024 Incentive Travel Index (ITI) reports that, overall, growth is projected for the incentive travel industry through 2026. Incentive travel buyers expect activity and per person spending above 2024 levels over the next two years.