Research / Reward Presentation and Attraction: A Biometric Experiment

Neuroscience

Reward Presentation and Attraction: A Biometric Experiment

by Incentive Research Foundation

Introduction

In Fall 2016, the Incentive Research Foundation commissioned Flying Horse Communications and its Chief Scientist, neuroscience expert Dr. Steven Genco, to conduct a first-of-a-kind experiment in the field of rewards and recognition. The experiment used biometric techniques borrowed from the science of neuromarketing. It was designed to gain additional insight into reward-earners’ preference in receiving rewards and to test two hypotheses about rewards themselves.

  • H1: Participants will exhibit a stronger response (attraction) to highly salient non-cash rewards than to cash at the unconscious level.
  • H2: After participants are given time to examine and consider their non-cash and cash reward options, most will choose a non-cash, hedonic reward over the equivalent cash.

The first paper in this series, Conscious and Unconscious Reward Preference & Choice: A Biometric Experiment, addressed the hypotheses. In this paper, we explore the question of reward-earner preference – at both the conscious and unconscious levels – in the presentation of rewards and recognition.

 

Background

A search for published academic research on the topic of how people prefer to receive recognition and rewards yields few results. However, Gallup and others have conducted numerous surveys over the past decade that demonstrate people are as unique in how they prefer to be recognized as they are in the types of rewards they prefer (Rath, 2004).

Classic philosophers and social psychologists from William James  to Abraham Maslow  as well as modern psychologists and behavioral economists from Edward Deci & Richard Ryan  to Paul Lawrence and Nitin Nohria,  acknowledge the deep human need for appreciation and recognition. Virtually everyone responds positively to recognition. Organizations proficient in recognizing their employees, customers and other key stakeholders enjoy significant productivity gains and greater commitment and engagement (Van Dyke & Mihal, 2011; Bartlomiejczuk, 2015).

Clearly, recognition matters a great deal, but its impact can vary widely depending on how it is delivered (Jeffrey, 2009; Shaffer, 2009; Dzuranin, Randolph, & Stuart, 2013; (Bartlomiejczuk, 2015; Kagel & Roth, 2016). In the first paper in this series, we explored reward type (cash vs. non-cash) to gain insight into unconscious preference and explicit choice. Our findings suggest both an unconscious and conscious preference for non-cash rewards, which supports a growing body of research that suggests non-cash rewards are more effective in driving performance gains, retention and engagement (Schweyer, 2017).

Our findings in this study (as reported in the first paper) confirm the importance of reward choice and individualization. It offers yet another warning against taking a one-size-fits-all approach to rewards and recognition. The experiments specific to this second paper in the series demonstrate that the same lesson applies to how rewards are presented. As other studies and surveys have found, some people prefer a great deal of pomp and ceremony when they are recognized, while others dislike the stage and would prefer a reserved and intimate presentation in front of their closest colleagues. Still, others prefer no show at all, just a verbal or written note of appreciation along with whatever cash or tangible non-cash reward that accompanies it.

 

The Experiment

For the experiment, 42 participants of varying age, gender and profession were recruited. In the reward presentation component of the experiment, each participant wore a headset and listened to a series of four realistic audio scenarios in which they received a reward during four different types of presentation: 

  1. A “Big Show,” in front of the entire company and delivered by the CEO
  2. “Little Show,” involving their work group and presented by the immediate manager
  3. “Peer-to-Peer,” in which immediate co-workers presented the reward with only the work group in attendance
  4. “Private,” in which a private, personal note was sent by the CEO accompanying the reward

While subjects listened to the presentation scenarios, they were biometrically assessed using two biometric techniques. 

  1. Pupil dilation: In brief, pupillometry measures arousal and interest based on involuntary expansion or contraction of the eyes’ pupils. Pupil dilation is described more completely in the first paper.
  2. BIS/BAS refers to two measures: Behavioral Approach System (BAS), which measures “appetitive motives,” and the propensity for a subject to “move toward something desired.” The Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) measures avoidance and the propensity for a subject to “move away from something unpleasant.” (University of Miami, n.d.) In more general terms, BIS/BAS measures a person’s underlying (i.e. unconscious) preference for different kinds of experiences, (Demaree, Robinson, & Everhart, 2005) including, in the case of this experiment, how people like to be recognized.

Our results are captured in Table One below.

Table One: Audio Presentation Summary

Though each of the presentation scenarios rated relatively well, our subjects’ unconscious reactions suggest a greater preference for a “Little Show” than for other forms of presentation. Importantly, when asked to rate the options (a conscious choice) “Little Show” is preferred by an even greater margin (86%). 

The “Peer-to-Peer” and “Big Show” presentations also rated well and closely with respect to each other. The “Private” presentation rated consistently last in its ability to “arouse” the interests of our subjects as measured by pupil dilation, and in explicit ratings. This supports the large body of research that suggests at least some form of public recognition is important to all reward earners, and it adds new evidence that smaller (and perhaps more meaningful) presentations may be more effective than big, company-wide events for most people.

Our experiment was relatively small with only 42 participants. Findings by demographic group in this experiment should be considered with that in mind. Table one results above show that men were evenly split in their attraction to the “Big Show,” “Little Show” and “Peer-to-Peer” presentation, while women were clearly attracted more to “Peer-to-Peer” both in their explicit ratings and as assessed by pupil dilation. More interesting perhaps, sales people were less attracted to the “Big Show” than non-sales people. 

Table Two below summarizes our findings by age group, role and gender. By age group, Millennials appear significantly more drawn to “Peer-to-Peer” presentations than other options. This is an interesting and potentially encouraging finding when juxtaposed with a large body of thought and research that suggests Millennials need more frequent recognition than other cohorts (Hershatter & Epstein, 2010). Our results also suggest that women are significantly less drawn to “private” reward presentations than men at the unconscious level.

Table Two: Audio Presentation Summary by Demographic

 

Conculsions

The audio presentation scenarios portion of our experiment revealed several unconscious and marked differences in preference for reward presentation scenarios using the biometrics techniques of BIS/BAS and pupil dilation. This is it important because we tend to use visible indicators such as age, gender and job type, to assume what people might want. Our results reveal new, invisible indicators that are strikingly important. 

In addition to those noted above, the “Big Show” option was less appealing to salespeople and women than to non-salespeople and men. It was the most disliked option by Millennials, but tied for first among Gen X. It is intriguing too, that those with high BIS where drawn to the “Big Show” presentation. These clues into the psyches of broad groups will help form additional hypotheses that can be tested in future research with larger numbers of participants. 

Ultimately, our results, including those from the visual components of the study, demonstrate a highly-individualized range of preference. They suggest again, in keeping with much prior research, that rewards and reward presentation are more effective when individualized to each person’s preferences. Thus, the most compelling indicator from these audio-based biometric experiments may be that reward presentation preferences can not be closely predicted on the basis of gender, role, age or even the combination of all three.

This conclusion should be further explored in future research with larger sample sizes. We expect future biometric-based experiments involving a larger number of subjects will bolster our findings with respect to wide variation in reward type and reward presentation preference.

 

 

THANK YOU TO OUR RESEARCH ADVOCACY PARTNER

 

Works cited
  • Bartlomiejczuk, G. (2015). How do Recognition Programs Impact Employee Engagement and How have Companies with a Large Global Footprint Structured such Programs to Drive Results? Cornell University, ILR School. Cornell University.
  • Demaree, H. A., Robinson, J. L., & Everhart, D. E. (2005). BEHAVIORAL INHIBITION SYSTEM (BIS) STRENGTH AND TRAIT DOMINANCE ARE ASSOCIATED WITH AFFECTIVE RESPONSE AND PERSPECTIVE TAKING WHEN VIEWING DYADIC INTERACTIONS. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115.
  • Dzuranin, A. C., Randolph, D., & Stuart, N. V. (2013, July/August). Doing More With Less: Using Noncash Incentives to Improve Employee Performance. The Journal of Corporate Accounting & Finance, 7.
  • Hershatter, A. & Epstein, M. (2010) Millennials and the World of Work: An Organization and Management Perspective. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25 (2), 211-223.
  • Jeffrey, S. (2009). Justifiability and the Motivational Power of Tangible Noncash Incentives. Human Performance, 22, 143-155.
  • Kagel, J. H., & Roth, A. E. (2016). The Handbook of Experimental Economics (Vol. 2). Princeton University Press.
  • Schweyer, A. (2017). Conscious and Unconscious Reward Preference & Choice: A Biometric Experiment. Incentive Research Foundation. Incentive Research Foundation.
  • Shaffer, V. (2009, January 1). Preference reversals in evaluations of cash versus non-cash incentives. Journal of Economic Psychology(30), 859-872.
  • University of Miami, Department of Psychology, (n.d.) BIS/BAS Scales. Retrieved from: http://www.psy.miami.edu/faculty/ccarver/sclBISBAS.html.
  • Van Dyke, M. V., & Mihal, M. (2011). The Human Science of Giving Recognition. Maritz.

 

Related Posts

VIEW ALL RESEARCH
Merchandise, Gift Cards, Event Gifting
IRF Webinar: Revisiting How to Maximize the Value of Your Incentive Destination 

IRF Webinar: Revisiting How to Maximize the Value of Your Incentive Destination 

During the webinar Revisiting How to Maximize the Value of Your Incentive Destination, the IRF led a panel discussion with incentive professionals who attended the September event to discuss the report and talk about they have applied these insights and harnessed the motivational power of destinations. 

Merchandise, Gift Cards, Event Gifting
IRF Webinar: 2024 Industry Outlook for Europe: Merchandise, Gift Cards and Event Gifting

IRF Webinar: 2024 Industry Outlook for Europe: Merchandise, Gift Cards and Event Gifting

Useful insights and benchmarks on merchandise, gift cards and event gifting. Focusing on European data, we discussed how incentive professionals are addressing current challenges, including rising costs and a tight labor market.